Recent years have seen an increasing influence on music theory of perceptual investigations that can be called phenomenological in the sense of Husserl, either explicitly or implicitly. The trend is problematic, particularly in what one might call its sociology, but it is also very promising. Potential or at least metaphorical links with Artificial Intelligence are especially suggestive. A formal model for "musical perceptions," incorporating some of the promising features, reveals interesting things in connection with Schubert's song Morgengruβ. The model helps to circumvent some traditional difficulties in the methodology of music analysis. But the model must be used with caution since, like other perceptual theories, it appears to make " listening" a paradigmatic musical activity. Composer/ performer/playwright/actor/director/poet can be contrasted here to listener/reader. The two genera can be compared in the usual ways, but also in some not-so-usual ways. The former genus may be held to be perceiving in the creative act, and some influential contemporary literary theories actually prefer members of this genus to those of the other as perceivers. The theories can be modified, I believe, to allow a more universal stance that also regards acts of analytic reading/listening as poetry.

[Footnotes]

[Footnotes]
1
(Ihde, 1976).
3
Kramer is a consulting editor for Music Theory Spectrum, volume 7 (1985),
"The Subconscious Language of Musical Time" (Rowell, 1975).
5
Miller (1984), p. 120
6
Generalized musical intervals and transformations (New Haven: Yale University Press, expected 1987).
7
Miller (1984),
10
(Dreyfus & Hall, 1982).
"Husserl's Anticipation of Artificial In- telligence" (pp. 17-19)
"Husserl's (and AI's) Problems" (pp. 19-27);
Otto Laske. Laske (1980)
Report Music and Mind, An Artificial Intelligence Perspective (San Francisco: Computer Music Association, 1981).
13
Raphael Eric Atlas (1983).
14
The American Heritage Dic- tionary of the English Language (New York: American Heritage Publishing Co., Inc., 1969), p. 63.
15
Fred Lerdahl and Ray Jackendoff (1983)
A Generative Theory of Tonal Music (pp. 264-269).
21
Freud (1955, pp. 64-65).
Joan Riviere (1952, pp. 68-69)
Earlier, Freud classifies many fehlleistungen as similar to Versprechen in mechanism (1955, p. 18;
Riviere, 1952, p. 29).
22
Oliver Strunk (1950, pp. 84-85).
(Zarlino, 1558/1968, pp. xviii-xxiv).
Mattheson (1739, p.6);
Heinrich Schenker's remarks about the Ursatz appear in Free Composition (Schenker, 1979, pp. 10-11).
23
Lerdahl and Jackendoff (1983)
25
(Gadamer, 1976, p. 36).
26
Miller (1984)
(Miller, 1984, p. 14).
(ibid.).
27
The passage is taken from "The Composer and His Message," a lecture delivered at Princeton University in the Fall of 1939.
28
(Lerdahl & Jacken- doff, 1983, p. 1.)
31
Lochhead and Fisher (1968, pp. 23-39).
32
Jonathan Culler's (1981, pp. 14, 107- 111)
33
Randall (1972).
Arnold Schoenberg's piano piece, opus 19, number VI (vol. 17, no. 2, Spring-Summer, 1979, pp. 17-24).
Benjamin Boretz influenced the journal over many years are only hinted at in his modest editorial apologia, "Afterward(: a foreword)," (vol. 22, Fall- Winter, 1983 and Spring-Summer, 1984, pp. 557-559).
Kenneth Gaburo is celebrated by a large number of contributions to volume 18 (Fall-Winter, 1979 and Spring-Summer, 1980, pp. 7- 256).
("Brain: . . . Half A Whole," pp. 215-256).
Robert Ashley appear in Formations, vol. 2, no. 1 (Spring, 1985), pp. 14-63.
34
Kay (1958, p. 68).
35
Barish (1981).
37
Lochhead and Fisher (1982).
39
Atlas (1983, pp. 26-27, 32-33)

References

References
Atlas, R. E. The diachronic recognition of enharmonic equivalence: A theory and its appli- cation to five instrumental movements by W. A. Mozart. Ph.D. dissertation, Yale Un- iveristy, New Haven, CT, 1983.
Barish, J. The anti-theatrical prejudice. Berkeley: University of California Press, 1981.
Barkin, E. Play it as it lays. Perspectives of New Music, 1979, 17(2), 17-24.
Bloom, H. The anxiety of influence. New York: Oxford University Press, 1973.
Boretz, B. Afterward (: a foreword). Perspectives of New Music, 1983/84, 22, 557-559.
Browne, R. The dialectic of good continuation in tonal music. Music Analysis, 1985, 4(1/2), 5-13.
Clifton, T. Music as heard: A study in applied phenomenology. New Haven: Yale University Press, 1983.
Cone, E. T. (Ed.). Roger Sessions on music. Princeton: Princeton University Press, 1979.
Cook, N. [Review of Clifton, Music as heard]. Music Analysis, 1983, 2(3), 291-294.
Culler, J. The pursuit of signs. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press, 1981.
Dreyfus, H. L., & Hall, H. Husserl, intentionality, and cognitive science. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press, 1982.
Forte, A., & Gilbert, S. E. Introduction to Schenkerian analysis. New York: W. W. Norton & Co., 1982.
Freud, S. Vorlesung zur Einführung in die Psychoanalyse. Berlin: Gustave Kiepenheuer Verlag, 1955.
Gaburo, K. Brain: . . . Half a whole. Perspectives of New Music, 1979/80, 18, 215-256.
Gadamer, H. G. Hegel's dialectic (P. C. Smith, Trans.). New Haven: Yale University Press, 1976.
Greer, T. A. Listening as intuiting: A critique of Clifton's theory of intuitive description. In Theory Only, 1984, 7(7,8), 3-21.
Hasty, C. Rhythm in post-tonal music: Preliminary questions of duration and motion, Jour- nal of Music Theory, 1981, 25(2), 183-216.
Husserl, E. The phenomenology of internal time consciousness (J. S. Churchill, Trans.). Bloomington: Indiana University Press, 1964.
Ihde, D. Listening and voice: A phenomenology of sound. Athens, OH: Ohio University Press, 1976.
Kay, G. R. (Ed.). the Penguin book of Italian verse. Harmondsworth, Middlesex: Penguin Books Ltd., 1958.
Keller, H. FA No. 1: Mozart, K. 421. The Score, 1958, 22, 56-64.
Kramer, J. New temporalities in music. Critical inquiry, 1981, 7(3), 539-556.
Laske, O. Toward an explicit cognitive theory or musical listening. Computer Music Jour- nal 1980, 4(2).
Lerdahl, F., & Jackendoff, R. A generative theory of tonal music. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press, 1983.
Lewin, D. Some investigations into foreground rhythmic and metric patterning. In R. Browne (Ed.), Music theory: Special topics. New York: Academic Press, 1981, pp. 101- 136.
Lochhead, J. The temporal structure of recent music: A phenomenological investigation. Ph.D. dissertation, State University of New York at Stony Brook, 1982.
Lochhead, J., & Fisher, G. The performer as theorist: Preparing a performance of Daria Semegen's Three Pieces for Clarinet and Piano (1968). In Theory Only, 1982, 6(7), 23- 39.
Mattheson, J. Der vollkommene Capellmeister. Hamburg: Christian Herold, 1739.
Miller, I. Husserl, perception, and temporal awareness. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press, "A Bradford Book," 1984.
Minsky, M. Music, mind, and meaning. In M. Clynes (Ed.), Music, mind, and brain. New York: Plenum, Press, 1982, pp. 1-19.
Narmour, E. Beyond Schenkerism. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1977.
Perloff, M. Postmodernism and the impasse of lyric. Formations, 1984, 1(2), 43-63.
Randall, J. Compose yourself—a manual for the young. Perspectives of New Music, 1972, 10(2), 1-12.
Riviere, J. A general introduction to psychoanalysis. New York: Washington Square Press, 1952.
Rowell, L. The subsconscious language of musical time. Music Theory Spectrum, 1979, 1, 96-106.
Russell, B. A history of Western philosophy. New York: Simon & Schuster, 1945.
Schenker, H. [Free composition (E. Oster, Trans. & Ed.)]. New York: Longman, 1979.
Schoenberg, A. Structural functions of harmony. New York: W. W. Norton & Co., 1954.
Strunk, O. Source readings in music history. New York: W. W. Norton & Co., 1950.
Tenney J. [Review of Clifton, Music as heard.] Journal of Music Theory, 1985, 29(1), 291- 294.
Zarlino, G. [The art of counterpoint, part three of Le istitutioni harmoniche, 1558 (G. A. Marco & C. V. Palisca, Trans.)]. New Haven: Yale University Press, 1968.
This content is only available via PDF.