Pitch-class sets (such as scales) can be characterized according to the inventory of possible intervals that can be formed by pairing all pitches in the set. The frequency of occurrence of various interval classes in a given pitch-class set can be correlated with corresponding measures of perceived consonance for each interval class. If a goal of music-making is to promote a euphonious effect, then those sets that exhibit a plethora of consonant intervals and a paucity of dissonant intervals might be of particular interest to musicians. In this paper, it is shown that the pitch-class sets that provide the most consonant interval-class inventories are the major diatonic scale, the harmonic and melodic minor scales, and equally tempered equivalents of the Japanese Ritsu mode, the common pentatonic scale, and the common "blues" scale. Consonant harmonic intervals are more readily available in these sets than in other possible sets that can be drawn from the 12 equally tempered pitch chromas.

[Footnotes]

[Footnotes]
1
Plomp and Levelt (1965)
Kameoka and Kuriyagawa (1969a).

References

References
Balzano, G. J. The pitch set as a level of description for studying musical pitch perception. In M. Clynes (Ed.), Music, mind, and brain: The neuropsychology of music. New York: Plenum. 1982. do. 321-351.
Cazden, N. Musical consonance and dissonance: A cultural criterion. Journal of Aesthetics and Art Criticism, 1945, 6(1), 3-11.
Danner, G. The use of acoustic measures of dissonance to characterize pitch-class sets. Music Perception, 1985, 3(1), 103-122.
Forte, A. The structure of atonal music. New Haven: Yale University Press, 1973.
Greenwood, D. Critical bandwidth and consonance in relation to cochlear frequency- position coordinates. Hearing Research 1991, 54 (2), 164-208.
Helmholtz, H. von. Die Lehre von der Tonempfindungen als physiologische Grundlage für die Theorie der Musik. Braunschweig: Verlag F. Vieweg & Sohn, 1863. [Translated as: On the sensations of tone as a physiological basis for the theory of music. 1878; second English ed., New York: Dover, 1954.1
Huron, D. Tonal consonance versus tonal fusion in polyphonic sonorities. Music Percep- tion, 1991, 9(2), 135-154.
Huron, D., & Sellmer, P. Critical bands and the spelling or vertical sonorities. Music Perception 1992, 10(2), 129-149.
Hutchinson, W., & Knopoff, L. The acoustic component of Western consonance. Interface, 1978, 7(1), 1-29.
Hutchinson, W., & Knopoff, L. The significance of the acoustic component of consonance in Western triads. Journal of Musicological Research, 1979, 3, 5-22.
Kaestner, O. Untersuchungen über den oetuniseinaruck unanaiysierter z,weiKiange. rsy- chologische Studien, 1909, 4, 473-504.
Kameoka, A., & Kunyagawa, M. Consonance theory, rart 1: Consonance or dyads. Jour- nal of the Acoustical Society of America, 1969a, 45, 1451-1459.
Kameoka, A., öc Kunyagawa, M. Consonance theory. Fart 11: Consonance ot complex tones and its calculation method. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 1969b, 45, 1460-1469.
Krumhansl, C. Cognitive foundations of musical pitch. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 1990.
Malmberg, C. F. The perception of consonance and dissonance. Psychological Mono- graphs, 1918, 25(2), 93-133.
Parncutt, R. Harmony: A psych oacoustical approach. Berlin: Springer- Verlag, 1989.
Plomp, R., & Levelt, W. J. M. Tonal consonance and critical bandwidth. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 1965, 38, 548-560.
Rahn, J. Basic atonal theory. New York: Longman, 1980.
Seashore, C. E. Psychology of music. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1938.
Serafine, M. L. Music as cognition: The development of thought in sound. New York: Columbia University Press, 1988.
Terhardt, E. Pitch, consonance and harmony. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 1974, 55, 1061-1069.
Vos, J. Purity ratings of tempered fifths and major thirds. Music Perception, 1986, 3(3), 221-258.
Wade, B. C. Music in India: The classical traditions. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall, 1979.
This content is only available via PDF.