The circadian rhythm regulates arousal and activity levels throughout the day and determines hours of optimal cognitive performance. Thus far, circadian fluctuations in face recognition performance received little attention in the research literature. The current experiment investigated the effects of time-of-day optimality on the ability to recognize faces and discriminate between the contexts in which where they were encountered. Morning- and evening-type participants (N = 91) encoded faces in a crime-related and a neutral context, either at their optimal or non-optimal time of day. Contrary to our hypotheses, neither face recognition nor source monitoring performance benefited from testing at optimal time of day. We discuss peculiarities of face processing that could account for the discrepancy of our findings with word recall and recognition literature.

Have you ever recognized a person but were unable to recall where you knew them from? This common experience reveals an important fact about the way our memory works: Familiarity is not always accompanied by the correct recollection of the memory source (Johnson et al., 1993). In everyday life the consequences of such memory source errors are mostly benign. However, in legal contexts the costs can be severe. For example, an innocent suspect may feel familiar to an eyewitness from mug shot viewings, repeated identification procedures, or because they were a bystander at the crime scene (Deffenbacher et al., 2006). Attributing familiarity to the incorrect source can lead to misidentifications and wrongful convictions (Lindsay, 2007), emphasizing the importance of studying factors that increase the risk of such memory errors. Time of day could be one such factor: Due to the functioning of our body clock, certain hours of the day are associated with the decline in cognitive performance, making us prone to memory errors (Schmidt et al., 2007). This observation motivated us to investigate whether testing at non-optimal time of day could be among the risk factors for errors in source memory for faces.

Our body clock generates the circadian rhythm which coordinates numerous aspects of our physiology, cognition and behaviour (Halberg et al., 2003; Refinetti, 1999). Among other things, the circadian rhythm determines optimal times for engaging in various activities and modulates cognitive performance levels across the day. Generally, we perform better at circadian peaks and worse at circadian troughs (Schmidt et al., 2007). The exact timing of circadian phases can vary across individuals, resulting in inter-individual differences in periods of the day that are optimal (Adan et al., 2012; Horne & Östberg, 1976). The so-called morning types perform best early in the day, whereas evening types peak in their performance in the evening hours (Goldstein et al., 2007; Levandovski et al., 2013). This pattern is known as the synchrony effect: Performance varies as a function of synchrony, or alignment of the time when a certain task is performed with the ongoing circadian phase (e.g., May, 1999; May & Hasher, 1998; Nowack & Van Der Meer, 2018).

Performance on long-term memory tasks also follows the standard synchrony effect pattern, with better performance at circadian peaks as opposed to circadian troughs. Recall of words and prose passages (Petros et al., 1990; Ryan et al., 2002), sentence and word recognition (May et al., 1993; Ryan et al., 2002), and cued recall performance in stem completion tasks (May et al., 2005; Puttaert et al., 2018) are better at the hours of the day that are aligned with participants’ chronotype. Such performance fluctuation can be explained by circadian cycles in arousal that are closely linked with the amount of attentional resources available to us across the day (Hirst & Kalmar, 1987; Necka, 1997). Conditions for encoding and retrieval of new memories are most optimal at circadian peaks, when arousal is highest. Circadian arousal troughs, by contrast, may not offer sufficient cognitive capacities necessary to engage in efficient cognitive processing, potentially leading to memory errors (Nowack & Van Der Meer, 2018).

The synchrony effect patterns in memory for encoded prose passages, sentences, word lists, word stems and pictorial stimuli are well-established (e.g. Intons-Peterson et al., 1999; May et al., 1993; Puttaert et al., 2018). However, this effect has not been studied before in the context of face recognition, which is a highly specialized function that differs from other types of recognition (Haxby et al., 2001; Kanwisher et al., 1997). In the current experiment, we made the first attempt to investigate the synchrony effect in the traditional face recognition paradigm.

Not all manifestations of memory rely on our attentional resources to the same extent. So-called implicit memory processes that operate outside of our awareness appear to be affected by the circadian rhythm in a different manner than standard recall and recognition tasks. For instance, priming seems to be affected by the circadian rhythm differently than cued recall. When explicitly instructed to use previously studied words on a stem completion task, participants usually show a classic synchrony effect pattern. Implicit memory tests, by contrast, generally show that performance is unaffected by circadian variations in arousal (Puttaert et al., 2018; Yang et al., 2007). In fact, one study reported a reversed synchrony effect (May et al., 2005).

To summarize, the circadian rhythm affects our conscious, intentional uses of memory. We perform better when tested at optimal time of day, whereas automatic influences of memory seem to be unaffected by testing optimality. A common explanation for this dissociation relates to the fact that automatic processes require little to no cognitive resources for their execution, and, therefore, can remain unaffected by many factors that generally impair cognition (Hasher & Zacks, 1979, 1984). By contrast, controlled, conscious memory processes heavily rely on available cognitive resources for their execution. Performance on tasks that rely on these processes is impaired when cognitive resources are limited (Smith & DeCoster, 2000). This also appears to be the case with time-of-day optimality: Reduced cognitive capacity at circadian troughs impairs performance on tasks with a stronger controlled component (e.g., May et al., 1993; Petros et al., 1990; Ryan et al., 2002), whereas automatic memory performance remains unaffected (May et al., 2005; Yang et al., 2007).

When providing testimony, eyewitnesses engage in a mixture of automatic and controlled memory processes. If presented with the culprit during an identification procedure, the eyewitness may be able to engage in an effortful, controlled process known as recollection in order to retrieve some qualitative information about the event (e.g., “I remember this man: He was holding the gun”). If recollection fails, the culprit may still be likely to be selected merely based on familiarity. This is a classic example of an automatic, effortless memory process (“I recognize this person – he must be the man from the crime scene”). In fact, automatic and controlled processes often work together in these scenarios to produce a correct recognition decision: The feeling of familiarity is complemented by recollection, resulting in an accurate identification. In other words, the ‘teamwork’ of automatic and controlled processes is beneficial when two memory systems point in the same direction.

For comparison, let us consider a scenario in which an innocent suspect becomes part of a lineup and feels familiar to the eyewitness. The suspect may have been present at the crime scene as a bystander, or their face may have become familiar from previously administered identification procedures or mug shot viewings (Deffenbacher et al., 2006). In these scenarios, a response based solely on familiarity could result in a misidentification. An accurate identification decision requires recollection of the context in which the suspect was encountered before (“They already showed me a mug shot of this man two weeks ago”).

Memory for the context in which information was encoded is often referred to as source monitoring (Johnson et al., 1993). Generally, source monitoring tasks are more difficult than standard recognition tasks, and impairing factors can have a disproportionate impact on source monitoring compared to recognition performance (for a review, see Mitchell & Johnson, 2009). This happens because the two tasks differ in the proportion of automatic and controlled influences of memory on task performance. Source monitoring relies to a large extent on controlled, effortful processes, whereas recognition decisions can be made based on a familiarity response that occurs automatically. As impairing factors affect recollection but leave familiarity intact, they produce larger impairments in source monitoring than recognition memory (Yonelinas, 1999).

In the current experiment, we investigated whether time-of-day optimality impairs face recognition and source monitoring performance. We tested morning- and evening-type participants either at their optimal or non-optimal time of day. Participants encoded face stimuli in two contexts: a crime-related context and a neutral context. At recognition, participants indicated whether they had encountered the face earlier in the experiment. We expected enhanced recognition performance when participants were tested at optimal versus non-optimal time of day. Specifically, we expected higher hit (hypothesis 1a) and lower false alarm rates (hypothesis 1b) at optimal than non-optimal time of day.

To experimentally recreate a situation that requires source memory judgments, we designed a task in which participants had to reject faces encountered in a neutral context and select faces they remembered from a criminal context. This procedure should parallel situations involving exposure to mug shots (Deffenbacher et al., 2006) or an innocent bystander prior to a lineup (Brackmann et al., 2019). Under such conditions, an incorrect ‘yes’ response to a face encountered in a neutral, non-relevant context can occur, if the face seems familiar but the memory for source is impaired. We expected that participants would be more likely to make such errors when tested at non-optimal compared to optimal time of day (hypothesis 2).

Participants

To determine the required sample size, we conducted a-priori power analysis for a one-tailed t test with G*Power v3.1 (Faul et al., 2007, 2009). Based on the earlier findings contrasting time-of-day optimality effects in explicit memory performance as opposed to priming (May et al., 2005), we expected an effect size of d = 0.52. We used an alpha error probability of .05 and a power of .80, which resulted in a required sample size of 94.

We pre-screened 346 individuals for their chronotype using the Morningness-Eveningness Questionnaire (MEQ; Horne & Östberg, 1976). We invited morning- and evening-type individuals for participation in the experiment. We managed to test 91 of the planned 94 participants (25 male, 66 female; age 18 to 33, M = 21.96, Mdn = 21 years), resulting in a power of .79 to detect an effect of the expected size. The sample included university students (n = 87) and members of the general public (n = 4). Among them, 57.1% were evening types (n = 52, MMEQ = 34.37, SDMEQ = 5.05) and 42.9% were morning types (n = 39, MMEQ = 63.77, SDMEQ = 4.46). We only included Caucasian participants to avoid cross-racial bias (Wilson et al., 2016). Forty-nine participants were tested at optimal time of day (38 female, 11 male, age 18 to 33, M = 22.18, Mdn = 21 years) and 42 participants at non-optimal time of day (28 female, 14 male, age 18 to 31, M = 21.69, Mdn = 21 years).

Design

We manipulated optimality (optimal vs non-optimal) as a between-subjects factor. Encoding context (crime-related vs neutral) and task type (recognition vs source monitoring) served as within-subjects factors. We manipulated encoding context by embedding face stimuli into crime-related and neutral scenarios. In the recognition task, participants indicated whether they had seen each face earlier in the experiment, irrespective of the context. In the source monitoring task, participants had to exclude faces from the neutral context, but indicate recognition for faces from the crime-related context. Proportions of yes responses to studied (hits) and unstudied faces (false alarms) from each context served as the outcome variables.

We used hit and false alarm rates to calculate discrimination accuracy and response bias. Discrimination accuracy (d′) indicates participants’ ability to distinguish signals (studied faces) from noise (fillers). A value of zero indicates zero ability to distinguish studied faces from fillers, whereas a value of 4.65 is considered an effective ceiling. Response bias (c), on the other hand, reflects participants’ threshold for deciding that they have seen the face before. A positive c value shows a bias towards saying no, whereas a negative c value reflects participants’ tendency to indicate that they have seen the face before (Green & Swets, 1966; Macmillan & Creelman, 2009).

Materials

Pre-screening questionnaire. We used the full version of MEQ (Horne & Östberg, 1976) to determine participants’ chronotype. The MEQ consists of 19 questions about participants’ sleeping habits, alertness levels at different points of the day, and preferred times for engaging in physically and cognitively demanding tasks (e.g., “At what time in the evening do you feel tired and in need of sleep?”). The MEQ score correlates with daily changes in melatonin and cortisol levels, body temperature (Baehr et al., 2000; Horne & Östberg, 1976), sleep habits and activity levels (Andrade et al., 1992; Bailey & Heitkemper, 2001; Duffy et al., 2001). The MEQ items in the pre-screening were intermixed with filler questions about food and caffeine consumption habits and sleep to provide additional support for the cover story.

Face stimuli. We selected 64 photographs with a neutral emotional expression from the Karolinska Directed Emotional Faces database (Lundqvist et al., 1998). All the actors were between 20 and 30 years old and were wearing the same grey T-shirt. When the stimuli were prepared, the actors were not permitted to wear earrings or eyeglasses, visible makeup, beards or moustaches. Thirty-two faces (16 male, 16 female) served as targets and 32 faces served as filler faces. Faces appeared against a background image depicting the respective context (see Figure 1). Importantly, for each of the actors, the database includes two versions of each facial expression taken at two separate occasions. Having two different photographs of the same person available allowed us to use different photographs at encoding and at test, which is a preferable way of testing face recognition performance (Burton, 2013). The appendix shows the counterbalancing plan.

Figure 1. Example of encoding stimuli presented in crime-related context (top row) and neutral context (bottom row).
Figure 1. Example of encoding stimuli presented in crime-related context (top row) and neutral context (bottom row).
Close modal

Procedure

Participants were recruited via advertisements at the university campus. The cover story held that the experiment concerned differences in food and caffeine consumption habits between morning- and evening-type individuals and their effect on memory performance. Morning sessions took place between 7:40 AM and 9:00 AM, and evening sessions were scheduled between 8:30 PM and 9:30 PM.

We instructed participants not to consume alcohol 18 hours prior to testing, avoid caffeine and chocolate on the day of testing and schedule participation only if they slept a minimum of six hours in the night prior to testing. Participants confirmed compliance to these criteria and signed the consent form at the beginning of the testing session. Next, we presented participants with the crime-related scenario:

Imagine waiting for a bus late at night. There has been an important football derby in your town and you notice a large group of football hooligans that are behaving violently. They approach a single fan of the other team and start insulting him. Very quickly the encounter becomes violent: the hooligans start pushing and beating him. You decide to call the police. You will now be presented with the faces of the hooligans. Pay attention – you may be asked to identify them later.

These instructions were accompanied by a context-cue: a photograph of a dark street with police cars and a policeman. Figure 1 depicts an example. After that, we informed participants that they would be presented with the faces of the hooligans and instructed them to pay attention as they may be asked to identify them later. Participants saw 16 faces, presented against the background that accompanied the imagery instructions, one at a time for 1 s followed by a 0.5 s interval.

Following a 5-min filler task (object search), we introduced the neutral context:

Imagine heading out to the supermarket on a rainy day. You drop off your beloved umbrella that you received as a gift from your grandmother at the entrance. After you finished shopping, you want to pick up your umbrella - but you realize that it’s gone! However, you notice another umbrella that is very similar to yours. Someone must have mistaken their umbrella with yours and taken it by accident. You will now see the people you’ve encountered in the supermarket while shopping. Pay attention – one of them might have your umbrella!

These instructions were accompanied by an image of a supermarket aisle. We told participants that they would now see the people they encountered in the supermarket and again instructed them to pay attention to the faces as one of them might have their umbrella. The faces were presented against the background of the supermarket aisle.

Following another 5-min filler task (object search) and a questionnaire about their sleep, food and caffeine consumption habits, participants received the recognition and the source monitoring tasks. In the recognition task, we reminded participants of the pictures they encoded earlier and asked them to indicate whether they had seen each of the faces in either context. Participants indicated if they had or had not seen each face earlier in the experiment. In the source monitoring task, we told participants that, unlike in the previous task, they should only select the faces previously seen in the crime-related context (i.e., the hooligans). Participants had unlimited time for their decisions. Participants received 5-euro gift vouchers or participation credit for their participation. We debriefed participants upon the completion of the data collection.

One participant was an outlier because they produced zero hits and false alarms on the recognition task and zero false alarms on the source memory task, whereas the proportion of correct responses on the source memory task was high (.88). We ran all analyses twice, once including the data from this participant and once excluding it. The pattern of results was analogous. We further report the analyses and descriptives including all data but exclude this participant from Signal Detection Theory calculations.

Table 1 presents mean proportion of yes responses to faces from each context across the experimental conditions. We ran a three-way mixed ANOVA to test whether optimality affected recognition performance (hypothesis 1) and source monitoring performance (hypothesis 2). We entered optimality (optimal vs non-optimal), test type (recognition test vs source memory test), context (crime-related vs neutral) as factors; proportions of ‘yes’ responses to faces studied in each context (i.e., independent of accuracy) served as the dependent variable.

Table 2 presents the results. Participants provided significanly more yes responses in the criminal (M = .48, SE = .01) than the non-criminal context (M = .41, SE = .02) and more yes responses to old faces on the face recognition test (M = .49, SE = .02) compared to the source monitoring test (M = .40, SE = .02). All the other effects were non-significant. These findings lend no support to hypotheses 1 and 2.1 Figure 2 shows participants’ performance on face recognition and source monitoring task at optimal and non-optimal time of day.

Explorative Signal Detection Theory analyses showed that sensitivity (i.e., participants’ ability to differentiate targets and non-targets) did not significantly differ between optimal (d’ = 0.81) and non-optimal time of day (d’ = 0.76), t(88) = .353, p = .608, d = 0.07. Similiarly, there was no significant difference between the referring response bias (i.e., the threshold for deciding that the target was encountered before) at optimal and non-optimal time of day (c = 0.95 and c = 0.74, respectively), t(88) = 1.150, p = .253, d = 0.24.

Table 1. Mean Proportion of Yes Responses [and 95% CI] to Studied Faces in Crime-related and Neutral Contexts and Non-studied Faces in the Face Recognition and Source Monitoring Task as a Function of Time of Testing.
  Time of testing 
 Context Optimal Non-optimal 
Recognition task Crime-related .52 [.46; .58] .55 [.48; .61] 
Neutral .42 [.35; .49] .47 [.40; .55] 
Non-studied .22 [.17; .26] .25 [.21; .30] 
Source monitoring task Crime-related .44 [.37; .51] .42 [.34; .49] 
Neutral .34 [.28; .40] .40 [.33; .46] 
Non-studied .17 [.13; .21] .19 [.14; .25] 
  Time of testing 
 Context Optimal Non-optimal 
Recognition task Crime-related .52 [.46; .58] .55 [.48; .61] 
Neutral .42 [.35; .49] .47 [.40; .55] 
Non-studied .22 [.17; .26] .25 [.21; .30] 
Source monitoring task Crime-related .44 [.37; .51] .42 [.34; .49] 
Neutral .34 [.28; .40] .40 [.33; .46] 
Non-studied .17 [.13; .21] .19 [.14; .25] 
Figure 2. Proportion of Yes responses to faces encoded in criminal (top panel) and neutral (lower panel) contexts in the recognition and source memory task (with 95% CI error bars and individual data points) at optimal and non-optimal time of day.
Figure 2. Proportion of Yes responses to faces encoded in criminal (top panel) and neutral (lower panel) contexts in the recognition and source memory task (with 95% CI error bars and individual data points) at optimal and non-optimal time of day.
Close modal
Table 2. Three-way Analysis of Variance of Proportions of ‘Yes’ Responses to Studied Faces by Optimality, Context, and Test Type
Factor SS F (1, 89) p ηp2 
optimality 0.077 01.031 <.313 .011 
context 0.492 18.473 < .001 .172 
test type 0.769 10.307 <.002 .104 
optimality x context 0.073 02.753 <.101 .030 
optimality x test type 0.009 00.122 <.728 .001 
context x test type 0.018 00.623 <.432 .007 
optimality x context x test type 0.016 00.545 <.462 .006 
Factor SS F (1, 89) p ηp2 
optimality 0.077 01.031 <.313 .011 
context 0.492 18.473 < .001 .172 
test type 0.769 10.307 <.002 .104 
optimality x context 0.073 02.753 <.101 .030 
optimality x test type 0.009 00.122 <.728 .001 
context x test type 0.018 00.623 <.432 .007 
optimality x context x test type 0.016 00.545 <.462 .006 

We tested whether face recognition and source monitoring performance varies as a function of time of day. Previous studies showed superior recall and recognition performance at those hours of the day that were aligned with individual’s peak arousal hours, as determined by the circadian clock (e.g., Anderson et al., 1991; May et al., 1993; Petros et al., 1990). The current experiment extends earlier work by investigating the synchrony effect specifically in face recognition. We hypothesised that face recognition performance would be superior at circadian peaks as opposed to circadian troughs (hypothesis 1). We also expected to find a synchrony effect for source monitoring performance (hypothesis 2). Surprisingly, our results show no evidence of such synchrony effect in either test. Participants performed very similarly across optimal and non-optimal testing sessions. These observations contrast with findings reported in previous studies that used non-facial stimuli to examine synchrony effects in episodic memory performance.

From a cognitive perspective, processing of faces differs from processing of other types of information (Bruce & Young, 2012; Robotham & Starrfelt, 2017; Schwartz, 2014). Faces are processed holistically (e.g., Richler & Gauthier, 2014) and are distinct in terms of allocation of attention during encoding. For instance, when presented with two faces simultaneously, encoding of one of the faces requires us to suppress processing of features of the other face, whereas this is not the case when we are simultaneously presented with two objects (e.g., Bindemann et al., 2007; Boutet & Chaudhuri, 2001; Palermo & Rhodes, 2002). Considering that circadian variations in cognitive performance are generally construed in terms of availability of attentional capacities (Valdez, 2019) and abilities of efficient allocation of cognitive resources (e.g., Nowack & Van Der Meer, 2018), the specifics of distribution of attention in encoding of faces may account for the fact that the circadian performance cycles are expressed differently in face recognition performance.

From a neuroscientific perspective, face recognition relies on specialized areas of the brain different from areas involved in other types of recognition memory (Schwartz, 2014). Areas of our brain responsible for memory functioning show oscillations that are autonomous from those generated in the suprachiasmatic nuclei, also known as the central pacemaker of our body. Even though these peripheral oscillators contribute to daily cycles in memory functioning (Snider et al., 2018), little is known about autonomous circadian cycles in the specialized areas responsible for face processing. The divergent pattern of daily fluctuations in face recognition compared to recognition of other types of stimuli could be linked to the fact that peripheral oscillations in the brain areas responsible for face processing function in a different manner. The combination of brain imaging techniques with the forced desynchrony protocol may offer promising discoveries about these dissociations.

Apart from general face recognition performance, we were also interested in potential negative effects of non-optimal testing on source memory. Source memory judgements can be important when eyewitnesses are presented with a suspect who is innocent but nonetheless familiar to them from another (non-criminal) context. In this situation, eyewitnesses have to attribute the suspects’ familiarity to the correct source. A failure to do so may result in the misidentification of an innocent person (e.g., Deffenbacher et al., 2006; Read et al., 1990; Ross et al., 1994). At a descriptive level, participants selected faces from the irrelevant context less often at optimal time of day than at non-optimal time of day. However, this difference was not statistically significant. Because general face recognition performance was also unaffected by testing optimality, we cannot assess whether these data suggest that source monitoring in general is unaffected by circadian variations in arousal. An experiment that tests source monitoring for non-facial stimuli may provide more comprehensive insight into whether our ability to discriminate encoding contexts can vary as a function of time of day.

One limitation of this study concerns overall low performance in the memory tests. Hit rates were low, though performance in target-absent trials was better, resulting in overall low sensitivity in both optimality conditions with small negative bias. It seems that participants found the task overall difficult, which may have masked possible effects of time-of-day optimality. Future studies might address this issue by increasing exposure duration, which should result in stronger encoding and better overall performance. Additionally, collecting confidence ratings and decision time data for recognition and source monitoring decisions in future studies would provide a more sensitive test of potential circadian variations in performance. Future research might include additional tests of cognitive performance along with long-term memory tests, allowing a comparison of time-of-day optimality effects across different cognitive domains.

Another limitation concerns the fact that faces from the crime-related context produced better performance than faces from the neutral context. Crime-related faces were presented first, possibly causing this difference in performance across the two contexts. Alternatively, participants may have perceived crime-related faces as more important. Even though the differences in memory strength for faces from the two contexts do not compromise our results, counterbalancing the order of contexts in future studies would allow to avoid differences in performance rates resulting from order effects.

Finally, encoding and retrieval in all our experiments took place in the same experimental session. As this experiment is the very first investigation of the synchrony effect in this context, it was most efficient to find out whether the circadian effects can be observed in face recognition performance in the first place. However, this design does not allow us to assess the effects of non-optimal testing on encoding and retrieval differentially. Future studies may address this issue by separating the two memory stages into different testing sessions and manipulating testing optimality for each of them separately, that is, by employing a testing optimality (optimal versus non-optimal) x memory stage (encoding versus retrieval) design.

To conclude, this work provides a first investigation into the possible circadian effects on face recognition performance. The current findings cautiously suggest that face recognition performance may not follow the standard synchrony effect patterns observed in memory for non-facial stimuli. It remains unclear whether this immunity to daily fluctuations in performance is a result of cognitive and neural mechanisms underlying face processing, which outlines important directions for future research. Aditional research is also necessary to test the possibility of the synchrony effect in older populations, which are known to have age-related decline in memory performance (Fitzgerald & Price, 2015) and are more prone to source monitoring errors (e.g., Benjamin & Craik, 2001; Schacter et al., 1991; Trott et al., 1999).

The authors report no competing interests.

S.Y., M.S. and L.H. designed the experiments. S.Y. conducted the experiments. S.Y., M.S. analysed the results. S.Y. wrote the main manuscript text. S.Y., M.S., and L.H. reviewed the manuscript.

We would like to thank Karpenko Mariia, Wedadi Babak, Harrison Jennifer, Reichel Lea, Knöpfel Dido, Völker Maja, Tix Ruth, Vieten Janine, Lück Severina, and Rau Aila for their help in piloting the materials and collecting data.

This research is supported by a fellowship awarded from the Erasmus Mundus Joint Doctorate Program The House of Legal Psychology with Framework Partnership Agreement 2013-0036 under Specific Grant Agreement 2016-1339 to Sergii Yaremenko.

Open data and analytic code are available online at https://osf.io/z8pv4/.

Appendix

Supplementary Table 1. Counterbalancing plan for face stimuli presented during encoding, face recognition and source memory task.
Optimal time of day Non-optimal time of day 
Encoding 
Crime-related scenario – Set 1 Supermarket scenario – Set 2 Crime-related scenario – Set 2 Supermarket scenario – Set 1 Crime-related scenario – Set 1 Supermarket scenario – Set 2 Crime-related scenario – Set 2 Supermarket scenario – Set 1 
Face recognition task 
Set 1A Set 2A Distractor Set 1 Set 1B Set 2B Distractor Set 2 Set 1A Set 2B Distractor Set 1 Set 1B Set 2A Distractor Set 2 Set 2A Set 1A Distractor Set 1 Set 2B Set 1B Distractor Set 2 Set 2A Set 1B Distractor Set 1 Set 2B Set 1A Distractor Set 2 Set 1A Set 2A Distractor Set 1 Set 1B Set 2B Distractor Set 2 Set 1A Set 2B Distractor Set 1 Set 1B Set 2A Distractor Set 2 Set 2A Set 1A Distractor Set 1 Set 2B Set 1B Distractor Set 2 Set 2A Set 1B Distractor Set 1 Set 2B Set 1A Distractor Set 2 
Source memory task 
Set 1B Set 2B Distractor Set 2 Set 1A Set 2A Distractor Set 1 Set 1B Set 2A Distractor Set 2 Set 1A Set 2B Distractor Set 1 Set 2B Set 1B Distractor Set 2 Set 2A Set 1A Distractor Set 1 Set 2B Set 1A Distractor Set 2 Set 2A Set 1B Distractor Set 1 Set 1B Set 2B Distractor Set 2 Set 1A Set 2A Distractor Set 1 Set 1B Set 2A Distractor Set 2 Set 1A Set 2B Distractor Set 1 Set 2B Set 1B Distractor Set 2 Set 2A Set 1A Distractor Set 1 Set 2B Set 1A Distractor Set 2 Set 2A Set 1B Distractor Set 1 
Optimal time of day Non-optimal time of day 
Encoding 
Crime-related scenario – Set 1 Supermarket scenario – Set 2 Crime-related scenario – Set 2 Supermarket scenario – Set 1 Crime-related scenario – Set 1 Supermarket scenario – Set 2 Crime-related scenario – Set 2 Supermarket scenario – Set 1 
Face recognition task 
Set 1A Set 2A Distractor Set 1 Set 1B Set 2B Distractor Set 2 Set 1A Set 2B Distractor Set 1 Set 1B Set 2A Distractor Set 2 Set 2A Set 1A Distractor Set 1 Set 2B Set 1B Distractor Set 2 Set 2A Set 1B Distractor Set 1 Set 2B Set 1A Distractor Set 2 Set 1A Set 2A Distractor Set 1 Set 1B Set 2B Distractor Set 2 Set 1A Set 2B Distractor Set 1 Set 1B Set 2A Distractor Set 2 Set 2A Set 1A Distractor Set 1 Set 2B Set 1B Distractor Set 2 Set 2A Set 1B Distractor Set 1 Set 2B Set 1A Distractor Set 2 
Source memory task 
Set 1B Set 2B Distractor Set 2 Set 1A Set 2A Distractor Set 1 Set 1B Set 2A Distractor Set 2 Set 1A Set 2B Distractor Set 1 Set 2B Set 1B Distractor Set 2 Set 2A Set 1A Distractor Set 1 Set 2B Set 1A Distractor Set 2 Set 2A Set 1B Distractor Set 1 Set 1B Set 2B Distractor Set 2 Set 1A Set 2A Distractor Set 1 Set 1B Set 2A Distractor Set 2 Set 1A Set 2B Distractor Set 1 Set 2B Set 1B Distractor Set 2 Set 2A Set 1A Distractor Set 1 Set 2B Set 1A Distractor Set 2 Set 2A Set 1B Distractor Set 1 

Note. Sets 1A, 1B, 2A, 2B contained 8 faces each. Each distractor set contained 16 faces.

1.

Sensitivity analyses conducted with G*Power v3.1 (Faul et al., 2007, 2009) showed that based on a sample size of 91, this statistical model allows to detect the effect size d = 0.36 with an alpha error probability of .05 and a power of .80.

Adan, A., Archer, S. N., Hidalgo, M. P., Di Milia, L., Natale, V., & Randler, C. (2012). Circadian Typology: A Comprehensive Review. Chronobiology International, 29(9), 1153–1175. https://doi.org/10.3109/07420528.2012.719971
Anderson, M. J., Petros, T. V., Beckwith, B. E., Mitchell, W. W., & Fritz, S. (1991). Individual differences in the effect of time of day on long-term memory access. The American Journal of Psychology, 104(2), 241–255. https://doi.org/10.2307/1423157
Andrade, M. M., Benedito-Silva, A. A., & Menna-Barreto, L. (1992). Correlations between morningness-eveningness character, sleep habits and temperature rhythm in adolescents. Brazilian Journal of Medical and Biological Research, 25, 835–839.
Baehr, E. K., Revelle, W., & Eastman, C. I. (2000). Individual differences in the phase and amplitude of the human circadian temperature rhythm: With an emphasis on morningness-eveningness. Journal of Sleep Research, 9(2), 117–127. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1365-2869.2000.00196.x
Bailey, S. L., & Heitkemper, M. M. (2001). Circadian rhythmicity of cortisol and body temperature: Morningness-eveningness effects. Chronobiology International, 18(2), 249–261. https://doi.org/10.1081/cbi-100103189
Benjamin, A. S., & Craik, F. I. M. (2001). Parallel effects of aging and time pressure on memory for source: Evidence from the spacing effect. Memory & Cognition, 29(5), 691–697. https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03200471
Bindemann, M., Jenkins, R., & Burton, A. M. (2007). A bottleneck in face identification: Repetition priming from flanker images. Experimental Psychology, 54(3), 192–201. https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169.54.3.192
Boutet, I., & Chaudhuri, A. (2001). Multistability of overlapped face stimuli is dependent upon orientation. Perception, 30(6), 743–753. https://doi.org/10.1068/p3183
Brackmann, N., Sauerland, M., & Otgaar, H. (2019). Developmental trends in lineup performance: Adolescents are more prone to innocent bystander misidentifications than children and adults. Memory & Cognition, 47(3), 428–440. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-018-0877-6
Bruce, V., & Young, A. (2012). Face perception. Psychology Press. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203721254
Burton, A. M. (2013). Why has research in face recognition progressed so slowly? The importance of variability. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66(8), 1467–1485. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2013.800125
Deffenbacher, K. A., Bornstein, B. H., & Penrod, S. D. (2006). Mugshot exposure effects: Retroactive interference, mugshot commitment, source confusion, and unconscious transference. Law and Human Behavior, 30(3), 287–307. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10979-006-9008-1
Duffy, J. F., Rimmer, D. W., & Czeisler, C. A. (2001). Association of intrinsic circadian period with morningness–eveningness, usual wake time, and circadian phase. Behavioral Neuroscience, 115(4), 895–899. https://doi.org/10.1037/0735-7044.115.4.895
Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Buchner, A., & Lang, A.-G. (2009). Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behavior Research Methods, 41(4), 1149–1160. https://doi.org/10.3758/brm.41.4.1149
Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39(2), 175–191. https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03193146
Fitzgerald, R. J., & Price, H. L. (2015). Eyewitness identification across the life span: A meta-analysis of age differences. Psychological Bulletin, 141(6), 1228–1265. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000013
Goldstein, D., Hahn, C. S., Hasher, L., Wiprzycka, U. J., & Zelazo, P. D. (2007). Time of day, intellectual performance, and behavioral problems in Morning versus Evening type adolescents: Is there a synchrony effect? Personality and Individual Differences, 42(3), 431–440. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2006.07.008
Green, D., & Swets, J. (1966). Signal detection theory and psychophysics. Wiley.
Halberg, F., Cornélissen, G., Katinas, G., Syutkina, E. V., Sothern, R. B., Zaslavskaya, R., Halberg, F., Watanabe, Y., Schwartzkopff, O., Otsuka, K., Tarquini, R., Frederico, P., & Siggelova, J. (2003). Transdisciplinary unifying implications of circadian findings in the 1950s. Journal of Circadian Rhythms, 1, 2. https://doi.org/10.1186/1740-3391-1-2
Hasher, L., & Zacks, R. T. (1979). Automatic and effortful processes in memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 108(3), 356–388. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.108.3.356
Hasher, L., & Zacks, R. T. (1984). Automatic processing of fundamental information: The case of frequency of occurrence. American Psychologist, 39(12), 1372–1388. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066x.39.12.1372
Haxby, J. V., Gobbini, M. I., Furey, M. L., Ishai, A., Schouten, J. L., & Pietrini, P. (2001). Distributed and overlapping representations of faces and objects in ventral temporal cortex. Science, 293(5539), 2425–2430. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1063736
Hirst, W., & Kalmar, D. (1987). Characterizing attentional resources. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 116(1), 68–81. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.116.1.68
Horne, J. A., & Östberg, O. (1976). A self-assessment questionnaire to determine morningness-eveningness in human circadian rhythms. International Journal of Chronobiology, 4, 97–100. https://doi.org/10.1177/0748730405285278
Intons-Peterson, M. J., Rocchi, P., West, T., McLellan, K., & Hackney, A. (1999). Age, testing at preferred or nonpreferred times (testing optimality), and false memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25(1), 23–40. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.25.1.23
Johnson, M. K., Hashtroudi, S., & Lindsay, D. S. (1993). Source monitoring. Psychological Bulletin, 114(1), 3–28. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.114.1.3
Kanwisher, N., McDermott, J., & Chun, M. M. (1997). The fusiform face area: A module in human extrastriate cortex specialized for face perception. The Journal of Neuroscience, 17(11), 4302–4311. https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.17-11-04302.1997
Levandovski, R., Sasso, E., & Hidalgo, M. P. (2013). Chronotype: A review of the advances, limits and applicability of the main instruments used in the literature to assess human phenotype. Trends in Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, 35(1), 3–11. https://doi.org/10.1590/s2237-60892013000100002
Lindsay, D. S. (2007). Autobiographical memory, eyewitness reports, and public policy. Canadian Psychology/Psychologie Canadienne, 48(2), 57–66. https://doi.org/10.1037/cp2007007
Lundqvist, D., Flykt, A., & Öhman, A. (1998). The Karolinska Directed Emotional Faces - KDEF [CD ROM]. Department of Clinical Neuroscience, Psychology section, Karolinska Institutet.
Macmillan, N., & Creelman, C. (2009). Detection theory. Psychology Press.
May, C. P. (1999). Synchrony effects in cognition: The costs and a benefit. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 6(1), 142–147. https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03210822
May, C. P., & Hasher, L. (1998). Synchrony effects in inhibitory control over thought and action. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24(2), 363–379. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.24.2.363
May, C. P., Hasher, L., & Foong, N. (2005). Implicit memory, age, and time of day: Paradoxical priming effects. Psychological Science, 16(2), 96–100. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.00788.x
May, C. P., Hasher, L., & Stoltzfus, E. R. (1993). Optimal time of day and the magnitude of age differences in memory. Psychological Science, 4(5), 326–330. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.1993.tb00573.x
Mitchell, K. J., & Johnson, M. K. (2009). Source monitoring 15 years later: What have we learned from fMRI about the neural mechanisms of source memory? Psychological Bulletin, 135(4), 638–677. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0015849
Necka, E. (1997). Attention, working memory and arousal: Concepts apt to account for the “process of intelligence.” In G. Matthews (Ed.), Cognitive Science Perspectives on Personality and Emotion (pp. 503–554). Elsevier. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0166-4115(97)80129-x
Nowack, K., & Van Der Meer, E. (2018). The synchrony effect revisited: Chronotype, time of day and cognitive performance in a semantic analogy task. Chronobiology International, 36, 1647–1662. https://doi.org/10.1080/07420528.2018.1500477
Palermo, R., & Rhodes, G. (2002). The influence of divided attention on holistic face perception. Cognition, 82(3), 225–257. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0010-0277(01)00160-3
Petros, T. V., Beckwith, B. E., & Anderson, M. (1990). Individual differences in the effects of time of day and passage difficulty on prose memory in adults. British Journal of Psychology, 81(1), 63–72. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8295.1990.tb02346.x
Puttaert, D., Adam, S., & Peigneux, P. (2018). Subjectively-defined optimal/non-optimal time of day modulates controlled but not automatic retrieval processes in verbal memory. Journal of Sleep Research, 28(4), e12798. https://doi.org/10.1111/jsr.12798
Read, J. D., Tollestrup, P., Hammersley, R., McFadzen, E., & Christensen, A. (1990). The unconscious transference effect: Are innocent bystanders ever misidentified? Applied Cognitive Psychology, 4(1), 3–31. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.2350040103
Refinetti, R. (1999). Circadian physiology. CRC Press. https://doi.org/10.1201/9781420048216
Richler, J. J., & Gauthier, I. (2014). A meta-analysis and review of holistic face processing. Psychological Bulletin, 140(5), 1281–1302. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0037004
Robotham, R. J., & Starrfelt, R. (2017). Face and word recognition can be selectively affected by brain injury or developmental disorders. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1547. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01547
Ross, D. R., Ceci, S. J., Dunning, D., & Toglia, M. P. (1994). Unconscious transference and mistaken identity: When a witness misidentifies a familiar but innocent person. Journal of Applied Psychology, 79(6), 918–930. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.79.6.918
Ryan, L., Hatfield, C., & Hofstetter, M. (2002). Caffeine reduces time-of-day effects on memory performance in older adults. Psychological Science, 13(1), 68–71. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00412
Schacter, D. L., Kaszniak, A. W., Kihlstrom, J. F., & Valdiserri, M. (1991). The relation between source memory and aging. Psychology and Aging, 6(4), 559–568. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.6.4.559
Schmidt, C., Collette, F., Cajochen, C., & Peigneux, P. (2007). A time to think: Circadian rhythms in human cognition. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 24(7), 755–789. https://doi.org/10.1080/02643290701754158
Schwartz, B. L. (2014). Memory for people: Integration of face, voice, name, and biographical information. In T. J. Perfect & D. S. Lindsay (Eds.), The SAGE Handbook of Applied Memory (pp. 3–19). SAGE Publications Ltd. https://doi.org/10.4135/9781446294703.n1
Smith, E. R., & DeCoster, J. (2000). Dual-process models in social and cognitive psychology: Conceptual integration and links to underlying memory systems. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 4(2), 108–131. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327957pspr0402_01
Snider, K. H., Sullivan, K. A., & Obrietan, K. (2018). Circadian regulation of hippocampal-dependent memory: Circuits, synapses, and molecular mechanisms. Neural Plasticity, 2018, 1–13. https://doi.org/10.1155/2018/7292540
Trott, C. T., Friedman, D., Ritter, W., Fabiani, M., & Snodgrass, J. G. (1999). Episodic priming and memory for temporal source: Event-related potentials reveal age-related differences in prefrontal functioning. Psychology and Aging, 14(3), 390–413. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.14.3.390
Valdez, P. (2019). Circadian rhythms in attention. The Yale Journal of Biology and Medicine, 92, 81–92.
Wilson, J. P., Bernstein, M. J., & Hugenberg, K. (2016). A synthetic perspective on the own-race bias in eyewitness identification. In B. Bornstein & M. Miller (Eds.), Advances in psychology and law (Vol. 2, pp. 241–270). Springer International Publishing. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-43083-6_8
Yang, L., Hasher, L., & Wilson, D. E. (2007). Synchrony effects in automatic and controlled retrieval. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14(1), 51–56. https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03194027
Yonelinas, A. P. (1999). The contribution of recollection and familiarity to recognition and source-memory judgments: A formal dual-process model and an analysis of receiver operating characterstics. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, And Cognition, 25(6), 1415–1434. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.25.6.1415
This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (4.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.

Supplementary Material