The present study examines the relationship between self-reported friendships with Blacks and implicit preferences for Whites relative to Blacks. There is considerable evidence that friendships relate to more favorable attitudes toward outgroups, however, the bulk of this evidence comes from explicit self-report measures. Using a sample of 123,445 participants that completed a Black-White IAT on the Project Implicit website, results indicate that participants reporting either childhood or post-childhood friendships with Blacks demonstrated weaker implicit preferences for Whites over Blacks. The size of this relationship was substantially smaller than found for explicit evaluations of Blacks.

Intergroup contact theory states that, under certain conditions, contact with outgroup members relates to more favorable attitudes toward the outgroup. This contention is supported by a large-scale meta analysis that found considerable support for the relationship between positive outgroup contact experiences and more favorable outgroup evaluations (Pettigrew & Tropp, 2006). Another meta analysis found similar effects for contact defined in terms of outgroup friendships (Davies, Tropp, Aron, Pettigrew, & Wright, 2011). The effects of friendship appear broad and applicable across many types of outgroups. For example, friendships relate to more favorable outgroup attitudes towards Muslims in the U.K. (Abrams, Van D Vyver, Houston, & Vasijevic, 2017), White Australians’ attitudes toward Aboriginal Australians (Barlow, Louis, & Hewstone, 2009), heterosexuals’ attitudes toward gay men and lesbians (Herek & Capitanio, 1996), and minority South African group members’ attitudes toward White South Africans (Swart, Hewstone, Christ, & Voci, 2011).1

Regarding the size of friendship-attitude relationships, meta analytic evidence summarizing results of 135 unique studies, found effects of roughly d = 0.50, 95% CI: [0.47, 0.54]; equivalent to r = .24) for friendships (Davies et al., 2011). The size of these effects are consistent with a more general meta analyses of contact-prejudice relationships (r = .225; Pettigrew & Tropp, 2006).

Although relationships between outgroup friendships and explicit outgroup evaluations are well established, less is known about the relationship between contact, friendships, and implicit preferences. Some evidence suggests that more favorable contact relates to less implicit ingroup favoritism. For example, White students’ favorable contact experiences with African Americans (Aberson & Gaffney, 2009, 2008; Aberson & Haag, 2007), non-disabled persons’ contact experiences with disabled people (Pruett & Chan, 2006), and heterosexual’s contact with gay men and lesbians (MacInnis, Page-Gould, & Hodson, 2017) all related to weaker implicit preferences favoring the ingroup relative to the outgroup. Specifically focused on friendships, close friendships with African Americans and Hispanics (Aberson, Shoemaker, & Tomolillo, 2004), friendships with Southeast Asian immigrants (Turner, Hewstone, & Voci, 2007, Study 1), and African Americans’ friendships with Whites in the U.S., and Muslims’ friendships with Christians in Lebanon (Henry & Hardin, 2006), related to weaker implicit preferences ingroups over outgroups. Additionally, a set of 12 studies included in a meta-analysis found an effect of r = .267, 95% CI: [.18, .35], (d = 0.55) for friendship-implicit preference relationships (Davies et al., 2011).

Although the studies previously cited found that contact/friendships related to weaker implicit ingroup favoritism, others found different patterns of results. For example, studies examining White participants’ contact with Asians (Prestwich, Kenworthy, Wilson, & Kwan-Tat, 2008), health care professionals’ contact with the obese (Teachman & Brownell, 2001), and contact with individuals with intellectual disabilities (Wilson & Scior, 2015) all found contact did not relate to implicit preferences. Surprisingly, others found friendships related to greater implicit preferences for the ingroup over the outgroup (Vezzali, Giovannini, & Capozza, 2012). Still others found the friendships related to implicit favoritism for outgroups relative to the ingroup. For example, a longitudinal study that examined African students in China found that friendships related to implicit preferences for Chinese over African faces (Qian, Heyman, Quinn, Fu, & Lee, 2017). Notably, sample sizes in these studies were relatively small, ranging from 30 to 326, with all but one study including fewer than 106 participants.

Implicit preferences for Whites over Blacks exist across numerous racial and ethnic groups. For example, large-scale investigations demonstrated that White (d = 0.91), Hispanic (d = 0.71), and Asian (d = 0.80) participants all implicitly favored Whites over Blacks (Nosek et al., 2007). Relevant to the question of the strength of the friendship-implicit preference relationship, it seems reasonable that since all participants evaluated Whites compared to Blacks, that there would not be differences between ethnic groups in how strongly friendships relate to implicit preferences. However, it also seems reasonable that White participants’ status as an ingroup in the comparison promotes resistance to changes in preference, suggesting that friendships effects would be weaker for Whites. Given these competing possibilities, the present work explores these relationships but does not offer a formal hypothesis.

Implicit preferences are often more weakly predicted by contact than are explicit attitudes (e.g., Aberson & Haag, 2007; Tam, Hewstone, Voci, & Kenworthy., 2006; Turner et al., 2007; Vonofakou, Hewstone, & Voci, 2007). More generally, both meta analytic and correlational evidence suggest that that affective measures of prejudice such as emotions and favorability appear more strongly related to contact than cognitive dimensions of prejudice such as beliefs or stereotypes (Tropp & Pettigrew, 2005, Study 1). It seems reasonable to believe that implicit preferences behave more like cognitive than affective measures.

Meta analytic work, however, found no difference between friendship-prejudice effects between implicit and explicit attitude measures (Davies et al., 2011). Notably, that work only included data from 12 studies that employed implicit measures. Among those studies that could be identified as including an implicit measure, sample sizes appear to be relatively small (n from 46 to 320). The small samples found in previous work suggests evidence for differently sized effects for implicit and explicit measures is limited. Given these issues, this study explores differences in the size of implicit/explicit relationships with friendship but does not put forward a prediction regarding differences in strength of effects.

Hypothesis 1. Participants with Black friends (either in childhood or post childhood) exhibit weaker implicit preferences for Whites over Blacks. This prediction is made for all subgroups.

Exploratory Research Question 1. Do effect sizes differ between ethnic/racial groups?

Exploratory Research Question 2. Do effect sizes differ between implicit and explicit measures?

The present work examines the relationship between reported friendships with Blacks and implicit preferences for Whites in relation to Blacks. Even if results are consistent with predictions, this does not suggest that friendships cause changes in implicit preferences. Such a finding might occur if individuals who less strongly implicitly favor Whites over Blacks were likely to explore friendships with Blacks. Similarly, it is possible that factors such as feelings of threat from Blacks or anxiety when interacting with Blacks promote both contact avoidance (i.e., fewer friendships) and greater implicit preferences for Whites over Blacks.

These data come from the Project Implicit Demo Website open dataset for the Race IAT (Xu, Nosek, & Greenwald, 2014; https://osf.io/52qxl/). I preregistered the analysis plan prior to downloading data (https://osf.io/yqbpj/registrations/). I note all deviations from the preregistration plan. The analysis code is available at https://osf.io/yqbpj/. The study includes data collected between 2/2/2015 and 9/26/2016 as this was the only period that included friendship measures.

The initial dataset included 2,079,623 cases, however this number was substantially reduced after exploring the data. After removing cases that could not explicitly be categorized as White non-Hispanic, Asian/Pacific Islander, or Hispanic, 580,017 cases remained. The preregistration indicated use of Maximum Likelihood estimation to account for missing data. However, 78.7% of the remaining cases were missing data on either the implicit measure or on one or both of the friendship measures. Absence of the implicit measure indicates that participants did not complete the task whereas missing friendship values reflect an unexpected question rotation pattern that did not ask every participant to indicate friendships. As this is too much data to impute, analyses of implicit preferences utilized only complete cases for the implicit and friendship measures (N = 123,445; this deviates from the preregistration). Among the remaining cases, missingness for the explicit preference measure was limited (1.8%). Analyses of explicit bias did follow the plan to use ML. There were no outliers or other cases removed.

Sample

Participants were 83.7% White, non-Hispanic, 10.2% Asian/Pacific Islander, and 6.1% Hispanic. Of these participants, 60.4% identified as women, 39.2% as men, and 0.4% as transgender. The median age was 22.8. The majority of the sample (80.8%) reported the United States as their country of residence.

Given the large expected sample size and the secondary analyses, there was no a priori power analysis. The present sample size provides power of 99% to detect effects as small as d = 0.03 for the overall analysis at α = .01. The White sample provides power of 99% to detect effects as small as d = 0.031. The Asian sample has power of 99% to detect effects as small as d = 0.087. For the Hispanic sample, power was 99% to detect effects as small as d = 0.120.

Analyses

Implicit preference analyses derived effect sizes (Cohen’s d) from an independent samples comparisons of participants that reported Black friends compared to those that did not. The lsr package (Navarro, 2015) extracted effect sizes and the MBESS package (Kelley, 2018) provided 99% confidence limits around the effect sizes. For analyses of explicit attitudes, I used the lavaan package (Rosseel, 2012) to calculate a standardized coefficient predicting the explicit measure from each friendship measure in separate analyses. These tests employed Maximum Likelihood for missing data handling. The standardized coefficient in this analysis is equivalent to a correlation. I converted the correlation to d for subsequent analyses. I computed Bayes Factors using the BayesFactor package (Morey & Rouder, 2018).

Measures

Friendships. Two items addressed friendships. The first read “I had a strong childhood friendship with a person who is …” and the second “I have had (or now have) a strong post-childhood friendship with a person who is …” Participants chose from a list including Asian, Black, Hispanic, Native American, Pacific Islander, and White. I classified all participants indicating a Black friend as having a friendship. Coding addressed childhood and post-childhood as separate variables. Additional exploratory analyses coded childhood and post-childhood friendships with Hispanics and Asian/Pacific Islanders.

Preferences. The implicit preference measure is the Race IAT using White and Black faces. The explicit preference measure is single-item feeling thermometer ranging from 0 (coldest feelings) to 10 (warmest feelings) toward Blacks. An exploratory analysis takes the difference between feeling thermometer measure ratings of Whites compared to feelings toward Blacks.

Hypothesis 1 predicted that participants with Black friends (either in childhood or after childhood) would show less implicit preference for Whites over Blacks compared to those who did not experience friendships. As shown in Tables 1 and 2, the data support this prediction with overall effect sizes of d = 0.132, 99% CI: [0.117, 0.148] and 0.160, 99% CI: [0.145, 0.175], for childhood and post-childhood friendships respectively. Implicit preferences were weaker for both those with childhood and post-childhood friends than those without. Bayes factors suggest considerable support for predictions with the smallest Bayes Factor (using default priors) for both tests exceeding a google.

Table 1

Means and Standard Deviations.

Childhood FriendsNo Childhood FriendsPost Childhood FriendsNo Post Childhood

M(SD)M(SD)M(SD)M(SD)

Overall 

 
Implicit Preference 0.296 (0.430) 0.352 (0.424) 0.298 (0.432) 0.366 (0.419) 
Explicit Evaluation 7.856 (1.994) 7.241 (1.928) 7.886 (1.978) 7.046 (1.879) 
TDiff 0.045 (1.387) 0.405 (1.616) 0.046 (1.373) 0.502 (1.670) 
Whites 

 
Implicit 0.304 (0.429) 0.358 (0.424) 0.305 (0.431) 0.373 (0.419) 
Explicit 7.873 (1.996) 7.280 (1.938) 7.900 (1.981) 7.073 (1.889) 
TDiff 0.091 (1.350) 0.455 (1.601) 0.089 (1.333) 0.567 (1.668) 
Asian 

 
Implicit 0.217 (0.441) 0.319 (0.423) 0.305 (0.431) 0.373 (0.419) 
Explicit 7.672 (1.932) 6.962 (1.798) 7.721 (1.921) 6.838 (1.751) 
TDiff –0.317 (1.609) 0.132 (1.644) –0.319 (1.654) 0.195 (1.614) 
Hispanic 

 
Implicit 0.285 (0.419) 0.342 (0.422) 0.283 (0.425) 0.353 (0.416) 
Explicit 7.853 (2.02) 7.250 (1.982) 7.875 (1.998) 7.131 (1.968) 
TDiff –0.147 (1.692) 0.246 (1.507) –0.148 (1.494) 0.318 (1.721) 
Childhood FriendsNo Childhood FriendsPost Childhood FriendsNo Post Childhood

M(SD)M(SD)M(SD)M(SD)

Overall 

 
Implicit Preference 0.296 (0.430) 0.352 (0.424) 0.298 (0.432) 0.366 (0.419) 
Explicit Evaluation 7.856 (1.994) 7.241 (1.928) 7.886 (1.978) 7.046 (1.879) 
TDiff 0.045 (1.387) 0.405 (1.616) 0.046 (1.373) 0.502 (1.670) 
Whites 

 
Implicit 0.304 (0.429) 0.358 (0.424) 0.305 (0.431) 0.373 (0.419) 
Explicit 7.873 (1.996) 7.280 (1.938) 7.900 (1.981) 7.073 (1.889) 
TDiff 0.091 (1.350) 0.455 (1.601) 0.089 (1.333) 0.567 (1.668) 
Asian 

 
Implicit 0.217 (0.441) 0.319 (0.423) 0.305 (0.431) 0.373 (0.419) 
Explicit 7.672 (1.932) 6.962 (1.798) 7.721 (1.921) 6.838 (1.751) 
TDiff –0.317 (1.609) 0.132 (1.644) –0.319 (1.654) 0.195 (1.614) 
Hispanic 

 
Implicit 0.285 (0.419) 0.342 (0.422) 0.283 (0.425) 0.353 (0.416) 
Explicit 7.853 (2.02) 7.250 (1.982) 7.875 (1.998) 7.131 (1.968) 
TDiff –0.147 (1.692) 0.246 (1.507) –0.148 (1.494) 0.318 (1.721) 

Note: Means and standard deviations for explicit based on complete cases. Lower scores on implicit relate to less preference. Higher scores on explicit relate to more warmth. TDiff = difference between White feeling thermometer and Black feeling thermometer.

Table 2

Effects Sizes, 99% Confidence Limits, and Bayes Factors for Total Sample and Subgroup Analyses.

dLower LimitUpper LimitBF10

Total Sample 

 
Implicit – Child 0.132 0.117 0.148 1.450 × 10105 
Implicit – Post 0.160 0.145 0.175 4.123 × 10168 
Explicit – Child 0.305 0.290 0.321 1.528 × 10592 
Explicit – Post 0.445 0.429 0.462 1.857 × 101216 
TDiff – Child 0.226 0.211 0.241 2.511 × 10326 
TDiff – Post 0.301 0.286 0.317 5.915 × 10575 
White 

 
Implicit – Child 0.127 0.110 0.143 3.478 × 1081 
Implicit – Post 0.159 0.143 0.175 1.251 × 10139 
Explicit – Child 0.295 0.278 0.311 1.692 × 10462 
Explicit – Post 0.437 0.419 0.454 3.751 × 10980 
TDiff – Child 0.233 0.217 0.249 1.232 × 10291 
TDiff – Post 0.321 0.304 0.338 1.800 × 10545 
Asian 

 
Implicit – Child 0.240 0.187 0.292 3.538 × 1028 
Implicit – Post 0.252 0.204 0.301 3.848 × 1037 
Explicit – Child 0.346 0.298 0.395 1.053 × 1075 
Explicit – Post 0.478 0.427 0.529 1.393 × 10139 
TDiff – Child 0.244 0.197 0.291 1.425 × 1037 
TDiff – Post 0.305 0.258 0.354 2.897 × 1058 
Hispanic 

 
Implicit – Child 0.135 0.074 0.197 2.554 × 105 
Implicit – Post 0.167 0.108 0.227 4.859 × 109 
Explicit – Child 0.296 0.234 0.359 2.614 × 1032 
Explicit – Post 0.381 0.318 0.445 3.756 × 1053 
TDiff – Child 0.236 0.175 0.238 1.674 × 1020 
TDiff – Post 0.286 0.225 0.348 1.468 × 1030 
dLower LimitUpper LimitBF10

Total Sample 

 
Implicit – Child 0.132 0.117 0.148 1.450 × 10105 
Implicit – Post 0.160 0.145 0.175 4.123 × 10168 
Explicit – Child 0.305 0.290 0.321 1.528 × 10592 
Explicit – Post 0.445 0.429 0.462 1.857 × 101216 
TDiff – Child 0.226 0.211 0.241 2.511 × 10326 
TDiff – Post 0.301 0.286 0.317 5.915 × 10575 
White 

 
Implicit – Child 0.127 0.110 0.143 3.478 × 1081 
Implicit – Post 0.159 0.143 0.175 1.251 × 10139 
Explicit – Child 0.295 0.278 0.311 1.692 × 10462 
Explicit – Post 0.437 0.419 0.454 3.751 × 10980 
TDiff – Child 0.233 0.217 0.249 1.232 × 10291 
TDiff – Post 0.321 0.304 0.338 1.800 × 10545 
Asian 

 
Implicit – Child 0.240 0.187 0.292 3.538 × 1028 
Implicit – Post 0.252 0.204 0.301 3.848 × 1037 
Explicit – Child 0.346 0.298 0.395 1.053 × 1075 
Explicit – Post 0.478 0.427 0.529 1.393 × 10139 
TDiff – Child 0.244 0.197 0.291 1.425 × 1037 
TDiff – Post 0.305 0.258 0.354 2.897 × 1058 
Hispanic 

 
Implicit – Child 0.135 0.074 0.197 2.554 × 105 
Implicit – Post 0.167 0.108 0.227 4.859 × 109 
Explicit – Child 0.296 0.234 0.359 2.614 × 1032 
Explicit – Post 0.381 0.318 0.445 3.756 × 1053 
TDiff – Child 0.236 0.175 0.238 1.674 × 1020 
TDiff – Post 0.286 0.225 0.348 1.468 × 1030 

Note: Child = childhood friendship. Post = post-childhood friendship. TDiff = Feeling thermometer difference score. Lower and Upper Limits correspond to 99% Confidence Intervals around Cohen’s d. Positive values reflect less implicit preference for Whites and warmer feeling thermometer ratings.

Research Question 1 asked whether effect sizes differ between ethnic/racial groups. Results show stronger effects for Asian/Pacific Islanders than for Whites and Hispanics (see Table 2). For all groups, implicit preferences are consistently weaker for participants reporting friendships with African Americans than for those with no friendships.

Research Question 2 explores differences in effect sizes for implicit and explicit measures (see Table 2). Feeling thermometer analysis produced a similar pattern of results with friendships related to more favorable outgroup evaluations. The explicit feeling thermometer measure demonstrated considerably larger effect sizes than the implicit measure. An exploratory analysis using the differences between feeling thermometer ratings of Whites minus the same measure for Blacks. These effects were weaker than for the Black thermometer on its own but remained substantially stronger than for the implicit measure.

Additional Exploratory Analyses

Reviewer comments on a preprint of the manuscript suggested two additional questions for exploration. This section addresses analyses limited to U.S. residents and generalization of contact effects. I did not include either of these analyses in the preregistration.

The first analysis addressed predictions regarding implicit preferences for U. S. residents (n = 99,783) only. Implicit preferences remained weaker for both those with childhood (d = 0.136, 99% CI: [0.119, 0.153], BF = 5.552 × 1098) and post-childhood friends (d = 0.168, 99% CI: [0.152, 0.184], BF = 1.021 × 10150) than those without. Although effect sizes were slightly larger than for the full sample, the magnitude of the differences is negligible (ranging from .004 to .008).

To address whether contact with one outgroup generalizes to implicit preferences toward African Americans, I examined White’s friendships with Hispanics and Asian/Pacific Islanders, Asian/Pacific Islander’s Contact with Hispanics, and Hispanic’s contact with Asians/Pacific Islanders. These analyses, shown in Table 3, demonstrate smaller effects than direct contact. As participants who had friendships with Hispanics or Asian/Pacific Islanders may also experience friendships with Blacks, an additional analysis focused on comparisons of participants that had friendships with Hispanics or Asian/Pacific Islanders but did not report any Black friends. These analyses demonstrate a small effect for Whites who had friendships with Hispanics but no Black friends in childhood only. Given the magnitude of this effect and the others shown in Table 3, results largely suggest that friendships with members of other non-Black ethnic or racial groups (absent friendships with Blacks) are not associated with more favorable implicit evaluations of Blacks relative to Whites.

Table 3

Effects Sizes, 99% Confidence Limits, and Bayes Factors for Contact Generalization Analyses.

dLower LimitUpper LimitBF10

White’s Friendship with Hispanics Overall 

 
Implicit – Child 0.109 0.091 0.127 3.568 × 1049 
Implicit – Post 0.127 0.110 0.143 1.182 × 1084 
White’s Friendship with Hispanics Overall (No Black Friends) 

 
Implicit – Child 0.039 0.012 0.066 11.45 
Implicit – Post 0.013 –0.012 0.038 0.026 
White’s Friendship with Asians Overall 

 
Implicit – Child 0.095 0.074 0.115 5.650 × 1028 
Implicit – Post 0.107 0.089 0.125 3.931 × 1049 
White’s Friendship with Asians Overall (No Black Friends) 

 
Implicit – Child 0.026 –0.004 0.056 0.152 
Implicit – Post 0.018 –0.011 0.046 0.045 
Asian Friendship with Hispanic Overall 

 
Implicit – Child 0.173 0.118 0.228 4.895 × 1012 
Implicit – Post 0.176 0.125 0.226 6.465 × 1015 
Asian Friendship with Hispanic Overall (No Black Friends) 

 
Implicit – Child 0.035 –0.043 0.114 0.067 
Implicit – Post 0.002 –0.073 0.077 0.033 
Hispanic’s Friendship with Asians Overall 

 
Implicit – Child 0.080 0.001 0.158 1.028 
Implicit – Post 0.131 0.061 0.202 3030.538 
Hispanic’s Friendship with Asians Overall (No Black Friends) 

 
Implicit – Child 0.019 –0.099 0.137 0.056 
Implicit – Post 0.006 –0.107 0.119 0.050 
dLower LimitUpper LimitBF10

White’s Friendship with Hispanics Overall 

 
Implicit – Child 0.109 0.091 0.127 3.568 × 1049 
Implicit – Post 0.127 0.110 0.143 1.182 × 1084 
White’s Friendship with Hispanics Overall (No Black Friends) 

 
Implicit – Child 0.039 0.012 0.066 11.45 
Implicit – Post 0.013 –0.012 0.038 0.026 
White’s Friendship with Asians Overall 

 
Implicit – Child 0.095 0.074 0.115 5.650 × 1028 
Implicit – Post 0.107 0.089 0.125 3.931 × 1049 
White’s Friendship with Asians Overall (No Black Friends) 

 
Implicit – Child 0.026 –0.004 0.056 0.152 
Implicit – Post 0.018 –0.011 0.046 0.045 
Asian Friendship with Hispanic Overall 

 
Implicit – Child 0.173 0.118 0.228 4.895 × 1012 
Implicit – Post 0.176 0.125 0.226 6.465 × 1015 
Asian Friendship with Hispanic Overall (No Black Friends) 

 
Implicit – Child 0.035 –0.043 0.114 0.067 
Implicit – Post 0.002 –0.073 0.077 0.033 
Hispanic’s Friendship with Asians Overall 

 
Implicit – Child 0.080 0.001 0.158 1.028 
Implicit – Post 0.131 0.061 0.202 3030.538 
Hispanic’s Friendship with Asians Overall (No Black Friends) 

 
Implicit – Child 0.019 –0.099 0.137 0.056 
Implicit – Post 0.006 –0.107 0.119 0.050 

Note. Child = childhood friendship. Post = post-childhood friendship. TDiff = Feeling thermometer difference score. Lower and Upper Limits correspond to 99% Confidence Intervals around Cohen’s d. Positive values reflect less implicit preference for Whites and warmer feeling thermometer ratings. White (n = 103,384), Asian/Pacific Islander (n = 12,562), and Hispanic (n = 7,499).

The primary goal of the present study was to examine the relationship between friendships with African Americans and implicit preferences for Whites over Blacks. Results indicate that participants who reported having Black friends demonstrated less implicit favoritism for Whites over Blacks. This finding adds to a body of literature suggesting friendships and, more generally, contact relates to more favorable outgroup evaluations over a broad range of measures.

Although these data provide substantial support for predictions, it is worth noting that the size of effects for the implicit measures are difficult to detect with sample sizes common to social psychological research. A sample of 1860 is necessary to detect population effects of d = 0.13 with 80% power. For 95% power, sample size requirements jump to over 3000. Comparatively, the observed explicit attitude effects were substantially larger and far easier to detect (e.g., a sample of n = 352 for power of 80% with d = 0.30). Future work studying friendship-implicit preference relationships requires substantially larger sample sizes than typically used in contact-prejudice research.

Exploratory analyses examined whether participant ethnicity moderated friendship relationships with implicit preferences. Interestingly, Asian/Pacific Islander participants demonstrated the strongest friendship effects. Speculatively, this may reflect differences in exposure to Blacks. The 2010 U.S. Census reports that nearly 2/3rds of Asian/Pacific Islanders are foreign born compared to 53% of Hispanics (U.S. Census Bureau, 2011, 2012). This suggests that exposure to Blacks is more novel for Asian/Pacific Islanders. Supporting this, within the sample, only 26% of the Asian/Pacific Islander sample reported childhood friendships with Blacks where as 38% of the Hispanic sample reported friendships. One area for further investigation is whether a lack of exposure enhances friendship effects.

Another exploratory question examined generalization of friendship effects. Specifically, whether friendships with members of other ethnic groups related to implicit preferences for Whites relative to Blacks. Initial analyses found some evidence suggesting friendships with Hispanics related to lessened implicit preferences for Whites relative to Blacks. However, when examining those participants who had friendships with Hispanics but not Blacks, generalization effects largely disappeared.

Although the present work cannot make causal claims that friendships reduce implicit preferences for the ingroup, some work does suggest a causal link. One study found more favorable attitudes toward gay men and lesbians after completing a friendship induction procedure (fast friends procedure; Lytle & Levy, 2015). However, another study that included an induced cross-racial friendships in the presence of a romantic partner related to more favorable explicit attitudes but did not find reductions in implicit preferences (Welker, Slatcher, Baker, & Aron, 2014). Although this work found no relationships between friendship and implicit preferences, the study included only 198 participants, suggesting the work was underpowered to detect differences on the implicit measure. As such, more work addressing friendship manipulation approaches is necessary to test a causal link.

Finally, as noted in the introduction, it is again important to stress that the present findings establish only a relationship between friendships with Blacks and reduced implicit preferences for Whites relative to Blacks. Although this is entirely consistent with a large body of evidence on the link between favorable contact experiences and reduced prejudice (e.g., Pettigrew & Tropp, 2006), the current findings should not be interpreted as causal. It remains possible that individuals who less strongly favor Whites over Blacks are more likely to engage in friendships with Blacks. It is also possible that other factors cause both friendships and reductions in implicit favoritism for Whites over Blacks.

The present work provides a large sample estimate of relationships between self-reported cross-racial friendships with Black and implicit preferences for Whites over Blacks. The data show relatively small (ds = 0.13 to 0.16) but stable differences in implicit preferences for those with friendship experiences compared to those without. Those participants with Black friends showed less implicit preference for Whites relative to Blacks. This relationship is substantially smaller than differences on an explicit assessment of feelings toward Blacks. Importantly, as the sample included over 100,000 participants, the effect size estimates show considerable precision that informs sample size planning for future investigations of friendship-implicit preference relationships.

These data come from the Project Implicit Demo Website open dataset for the Race IAT (Xu, Nosek, & Greenwald, 2014; https://osf.io/52qxl/. DOI: https://doi.org/10.5334/jopd.ac).

Throughout this manuscript, I used the term African American when discussing work completed in the United States, but the term Black to discuss international work and results from samples that were not exclusively U.S.-based.

The author has no competing interests to declare.

1
Aberson
,
C. L.
, &
Gaffney
,
A. M.
(
2009
).
An integrated threat model of explicit and implicit preferences
.
European Journal of Social Psychology
,
39
,
808
830
. DOI:
2
Aberson
,
C. L.
, &
Haag
,
S. C.
(
2007
).
Contact, perspective taking, and anxiety as predictors of stereotype endorsement, explicit attitudes, and implicit preferences
.
Group Processes & Intergroup Relations
,
10
,
179
201
. DOI:
3
Aberson
,
C. L.
,
Porter
,
M. K.
, &
Gaffney
,
A. M.
(
2008
).
Friendships influence Hispanic students’ implicit attitudes toward white non-Hispanics relative to African Americans
.
Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences
,
30
,
544
556
. DOI:
4
Aberson
,
C. L.
,
Shoemaker
,
C.
, &
Tomolillo
,
C.
(
2004
).
Implicit bias and contact: The role of interethnic friendships
.
The Journal of Social Psychology
,
144
,
335
347
. DOI:
5
Abrams
,
D.
,
Van de Vyver
,
J.
,
Houston
,
D. M.
, &
Vasiljevic
,
M.
(
2017
).
Does terror defeat contact? intergroup contact and prejudice toward Muslims before and after the London bombings
.
Peace and Conflict: Journal of Peace Psychology
,
23
,
260
268
. DOI:
6
Barlow
,
F. K.
,
Louis
,
W. R.
, &
Hewstone
,
M.
(
2009
).
Rejected! Cognitions of rejection and intergroup anxiety as mediators of the impact of cross-group friendships on prejudice
.
British Journal of Social Psychology
,
48
,
389
405
. DOI:
7
Davies
,
K.
,
Tropp
,
L. R.
,
Aron
,
A.
,
Pettigrew
,
T. F.
, &
Wright
,
S. C.
(
2011
).
Cross-group friendships and intergroup attitudes: A meta-analytic review
.
Personality and Social Psychology Review
,
15
,
332
351
. DOI:
8
Henry
,
P. J.
, &
Hardin
,
C. D.
(
2006
).
The contact hypothesis revisited: Status bias in the reduction of implicit prejudice in the United States and Lebanon
.
Psychological Science
,
17
,
862
868
. DOI:
9
Herek
,
G. M.
, &
Capitanio
,
J. P.
(
1996
).
“Some of my best friends”: Intergroup contact, concealable stigma, and heterosexuals’ attitudes toward gay men and lesbians
.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin
,
22
,
412
424
. DOI:
10
Kelley
,
K.
(
2018
).
MBESS: The MBESS R Package. R package version 4.4.3
. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=MBESS.
11
Lytle
,
A.
, &
Levy
,
S. R.
(
2015
).
Reducing heterosexuals’ prejudice toward gay men and lesbian women via an induced cross-orientation friendship
.
Psychology of Sexual Orientation and Gender Diversity
,
2
,
447
455
. DOI:
12
MacInnis
,
C. C.
,
Page-Gould
,
E.
, &
Hodson
,
G.
(
2017
).
Multilevel intergroup contact and antigay prejudice (explicit and implicit): Evidence of contextual contact benefits in a less visible group domain
.
Social Psychological and Personality Science
,
8
,
243
251
. DOI:
13
Morey
,
R. D.
, &
Rouder
,
J. N.
(
2018
).
BayesFactor: Computation of Bayes Factors for common designs. R package version 0.9.12-4.1
. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=BayesFactor.
14
Navarro
,
D. J.
(
2015
) Learning statistics with R: A tutorial for psychology students and other beginners. (Version 0.5)
Adelaide, Australia
:
University of Adelaide
.
15
Nosek
,
B. A.
,
Smyth
,
F. L.
,
Hansen
,
J. J.
,
Devos
,
T.
,
Lindner
,
N. M.
,
Ranganath
,
K. A.
,
Banaji
,
M. R.
, et al. (
2007
).
Pervasiveness and correlates of implicit attitudes and stereotypes
.
European Review of Social Psychology
,
18
,
36
88
. DOI:
16
Pettigrew
,
T. F.
, &
Tropp
,
L. R.
(
2006
).
A meta-analytic test of intergroup contact theory
.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology
,
90
,
751
783
. DOI:
17
Prestwich
,
A.
,
Kenworthy
,
J. B.
,
Wilson
,
M.
, &
Kwan-Tat
,
N.
(
2008
).
Differential relations between two types of contact and implicit and explicit racial attitudes
.
British Journal of Social Psychology
,
47
,
575
588
. DOI:
18
Pruett
,
S. R.
, &
Chan
,
F.
(
2006
).
The development and psychometric validation of the disability attitude implicit association test
.
Rehabilitation Psychology
,
51
,
202
213
. DOI:
19
Qian
,
M. K.
,
Heyman
,
G. D.
,
Quinn
,
P. C.
,
Fu
,
G.
, &
Lee
,
K.
(
2017
).
When the majority becomes the minority: A longitudinal study of the effects of immersive experience with racial out-group members on implicit and explicit racial biases
.
Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology
,
48
,
914
930
. DOI:
20
Rosseel
,
Y.
(
2012
).
lavaan: An R Package for Structural Equation Modeling
.
Journal of Statistical Software
,
48
,
1
36
. http://www.jstatsoft.org/v48/i02/. DOI:
21
Swart
,
H.
,
Hewstone
,
M.
,
Christ
,
O.
, &
Voci
,
A.
(
2011
).
Affective mediators of intergroup contact: A three-wave longitudinal study in South Africa
.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology
,
101
,
1221
1238
. DOI:
22
Tam
,
T.
,
Hewstone
,
M.
,
Harwood
,
J.
,
Voci
,
A.
, &
Kenworthy
,
J.
(
2006
).
Intergroup contact and grandparent-grandchild communication: The effects of self-disclosure on implicit and explicit biases against older people
.
Group Processes & Intergroup Relations
,
9
,
413
429
. DOI:
23
Teachman
,
B. A.
, &
Brownell
,
K. D.
(
2001
).
Implicit anti-fat bias among health professionals: Is anyone immune?
International Journal of Obesity
,
25
,
1525
1531
. DOI:
24
Tropp
,
L. R.
, &
Pettigrew
,
T. F.
(
2005
).
Differential relationships between intergroup contact and affective and cognitive dimensions of prejudice
.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin
,
31
,
1145
1158
. DOI:
25
Turner
,
R. N.
,
Hewstone
,
M.
, &
Voci
,
A.
(
2007
).
Reducing explicit and implicit outgroup prejudice via direct and extended contact: The mediating role of self-disclosure and intergroup anxiety
.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology
,
93
,
369
388
. DOI:
26
U.S. Census Bureau
. (
2011
,
May
).
The Hispanic Population: 2010
(Report No. C2010BR-04).
Washington, DC
:
U.S. Government Printing Office
.
27
U.S. Census Bureau
. (
2012
,
May
).
The Asian Population: 2010
(Report No. C2010BR-11).
Washington, DC
:
U.S. Government Printing Office
.
28
Vezzali
,
L.
,
Giovannini
,
D.
, &
Capozza
,
D.
(
2012
).
Social antecedents of children’s implicit prejudice: Direct contact, extended contact, explicit and implicit teachers’ prejudice
.
European Journal of Developmental Psychology
,
9
,
569
581
. DOI:
29
Vonofakou
,
C.
,
Hewstone
,
M.
, &
Voci
,
A.
(
2007
).
Contact with out-group friends as a predictor of meta-attitudinal strength and accessibility of attitudes toward gay men
.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology
,
92
,
804
820
. DOI:
30
Welker
,
K. M.
,
Slatcher
,
R. B.
,
Baker
,
L.
, &
Aron
,
A.
(
2014
).
Creating positive out-group attitudes through intergroup couple friendships and implications for compassionate love
.
Journal of Social and Personal Relationships
,
31
,
706
725
. DOI:
31
Wilson
,
M. C.
, &
Scior
,
K.
(
2015
).
Implicit preferences towards people with intellectual disabilities: Their relationship with explicit attitudes, social distance, emotions and contact
.
PLoS ONE
,
10
, e0137902. DOI:
32
Xu
,
K.
,
Nosek
,
B. A.
, &
Greenwald
,
A. G.
(
2014
).
Psychology data from the Race Implicit Association Test on the Project Implicit Demo website
.
Journal of Open Psychology Data
,
2
, e3. DOI:

The author(s) of this paper chose the Open Review option, and the peer review comments are available at: http://doi.org/10.1525/collabra.195.pr

This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (CC-BY 4.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited. See http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.